题目内容

Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so, I whispered to my uncle and his wife, "Just want you to know: I think what I got you is really cool, so just tell me you like it, no matter what, okay" I know that sounds rude, but there’s another Christmas custom in my family: we give each other weird gifts. There is a sweet reason for this. My grandparents grew up during the Depression, and there were years when they had no gifts at all. So my grandmother and her siblings(兄弟姐妹) would gift-wrap their old socks and clothes, just so they had something to open on Christmas. Pretend presents were better than none at all. My grandmother never really got over those early years, so, for the rest of her life, she went a little crazy at the holidays. She’d start buying gifts in October. It didn’t matter what it was. Socks, toothbrushes, used paperbacks she’d read but didn’t like, all went under the tree. Contents of catalog "mystery boxes" meant we spent another hour unwrapping presents. One of my more memorable gifts: a single piece of clear plastic labeled "face shield." I was apparently to hold it in front of my eyes when I used hair spray. We all thanked Grandma greatly no matter what we got. As a little kid, this pattern of gratitude for the terrible presents puzzled me; it took a long time for me to understand it was all right to laugh at some of her gifts. Now I don’t really know if my aunt and uncle actually liked the gift I gave them. They said they did, but since I coached them to tell me they love it, I’ll never really know if that was the truth. All of these make me think of the work of McGill professor Victoria Talwar. As an expert in children’s lying behavior, Talwar has been studying how kids respond to unwanted gifts. When they get a gift they hate, can they still thank someone and pretend to love it Talwar tests kids’ ability to do this, by asking kids to pick a toy they want; if they win a game, they get the chosen toy. There are plastic horses, a small car, a few other items, including an unwrapped, dirty, worn, used bar of soap. At some point in the game, there’s a switch in the adults who play with the kids. So, instead of giving the child her chosen toy, the late-arriving adult gives the child the soap. Then, the researchers watch what happens. 68% of kids, aged 3 to 11, will spontaneously say they love the gift of old ugly soap. The older they are, the more likely they are to say a white lie about the gift. And if parents encourage the children to say how much they like the present, the percentage of kids lying about the gift increases to 87%. At this point, some may be saying that a white lie isn’t a lie. That’s because you are looking at lying from the adult perspective--that lies are acceptable, when told with the intent of helping someone, or protecting another’s feelings. But kids don’t think of lying in the same way. For them, the intent behind a lie--for good or for ill--is irrelevant. It is so irrelevant that, for very young kids, you can’t even lie by accident. Someone who gives out wrong information, but believed it to be true, is still a liar in these kids’ book. Kids just don’t believe that lying comes in shades of white or gray. Lying is much simpler than that: lying is telling somebody something that isn’t so; lying is really bad; and lying gets you punished. And if it gets you punished, you shouldn’t do it. In Talwar’s lab, parents have literally cheered to hear their kids lie about how great it is to have received the old soap. The parents have pride over their children’s knowing the socially appropriate response. Talwar’s regularly amazed by this. The parents never even seem to realize that the child told a lie. They never want to scold the child afterwards, or talk about the kid’s behavior. Regardless of the parents’ pride, the kids aren’t happy about their successfully lying. Instead, it can be torture for them. I was at Talwar’s lab when she was doing a version of the unwanted gift experiment with kids in the first and second grades. Watching kid after kid react to that gross bar of soap, I could really see how emotionally difficult it is for kids to tell a white lie. The kids were disappointed when they were handed the soap, but that was nothing compared to the discomfort they showed while having to lie about liking it. They are uneasy. Some looked like they were going to cry. It was simply painful to watch. Indeed, Talwar has found that some kids just can’t even bring themselves to say something nice about the present. About 20% of 11-year-olds just refuse to tell a white lie about that unwanted gift--even after their parents encouraged them to do so. And about 14% of kids still won’t tell a white lie, even after their parents specifically explained the prosocial (亲社会的) reasons to tell the lie. These kids just can’t reconcile the disconnect between knowing how bad lying is, and being told they should now lie. Talwar cautions that we need to recognize that, at least from the kids’ point of view, white lies really are still lies. We should take care to explain the motivation behind the untruth--that we want to protect the other person’s feelings. Kids may still fail to completely understand the distinction, but at least it will encourage them to think about others’ feelings when they act. And we need to reassure children that they won’t be punished for a specific white lie--because they did something nice for someone else. Talwar also warns that we adults should pay attention to our own use of white lies. Kids notice these untruths-and that we rarely get punished for them. If kids believe that we regularly lie to get out of uncomfortable social situations, they are more likely to adopt a similar strategy of lying. If we don’t watch it, we could unintentionally be giving kids yet another present: a license to lie. How did the author’s grandmother and her siblings manage to receive Christmas gifts during the Depression’

A. They earned money to buy gifts for each other.
B. They made pretend presents with old things.
C. They started buying gifts in October.
D. They gave each other weird gifts.

查看答案
更多问题

Higher education has a responsibility to provide a workforce with the design capability and high-level technical design skills to generate growth in (67) industries with the potential to support the UK’s (68) health. A Higher Ambitions report states: "Britain must rebuild its economic growth and (69) in the skills and competitive strengths that will underwrite a(n) (70) recovery and balanced future economy." (71) , it specifically calls for "enhanced support for the Stem subjects-- (72) in the sciences, technology, engineering and mathematics--and other skills that underwrite this country’s competitive (73) ". The exclusion of design subjects from Stem (74) the common nature of these subjects, (75) science in the employment market and the strategic significance of design as a(n) (76) to the economy. The government’s Digital Britain report demonstrates that the creative industries sector is in a good position to (77) Britain out of recession. Lord Drayson, minister for science and innovation, has said: "The next few years will (78) the UK’s innovation capability,and while the recession may (79) challenges, it will also provide (80) ." Britain’s creative practitioners are (81) high demand globally. The National Endowment for Science, Technology and the Arts (82) that the creative industries sector will grow by an average of 4%, more than double the rest of the economy. The Design Council’s added value research showed that companies that used design to add value to their products or services had a higher (83) than competitors on profitability, turnover and market share. De sign is (84) allied to innovation and is widely (85) as a crucial ingredient in maintaining competitiveness in (86) challenging markets.

A. key
B. inferior
C. minor
D. central

Questions 22 to 25 are based on the conversation you have just heard. What do we learn about a"smart"car according to the woman

A. It uses new technology.
B. It is installed with a robot.
C. It is as clever as human.
D. It can talk with people.

Should A Kid Be Guided to Tell a White Lie It’s my family’s tradition to exchange girls on Christmas Eve. Before we did so, I whispered to my uncle and his wife, "Just want you to know: I think what I got you is really cool, so just tell me you like it, no matter what, okay" I know that sounds rude, but there’s another Christmas custom in my family: we give each other weird gifts. There is a sweet reason for this. My grandparents grew up during the Depression, and there were years when they had no gifts at all. So my grandmother and her siblings(兄弟姐妹) would gift-wrap their old socks and clothes, just so they had something to open on Christmas. Pretend presents were better than none at all. My grandmother never really got over those early years, so, for the rest of her life, she went a little crazy at the holidays. She’d start buying gifts in October. It didn’t matter what it was. Socks, toothbrushes, used paperbacks she’d read but didn’t like, all went under the tree. Contents of catalog "mystery boxes" meant we spent another hour unwrapping presents. One of my more memorable gifts: a single piece of clear plastic labeled "face shield." I was apparently to hold it in front of my eyes when I used hair spray. We all thanked Grandma greatly no matter what we got. As a little kid, this pattern of gratitude for the terrible presents puzzled me; it took a long time for me to understand it was all right to laugh at some of her gifts. Now I don’t really know if my aunt and uncle actually liked the gift I gave them. They said they did, but since I coached them to tell me they love it, I’ll never really know if that was the truth. All of these make me think of the work of McGill professor Victoria Talwar. As an expert in children’s lying behavior, Talwar has been studying how kids respond to unwanted gifts. When they get a gift they hate, can they still thank someone and pretend to love it Talwar tests kids’ ability to do this, by asking kids to pick a toy they want; if they win a game, they get the chosen toy. There are plastic horses, a small car, a few other items, including an unwrapped, dirty, worn, used bar of soap. At some point in the game, there’s a switch in the adults who play with the kids. So, instead of giving the child her chosen toy, the late-arriving adult gives the child the soap. Then, the researchers watch what happens. 68% of kids, aged 3 to 11, will spontaneously say they love the gift of old ugly soap. The older they are, the more likely they are to say a white lie about the gift. And if parents encourage the children to say how much they like the present, the percentage of kids lying about the gift increases to 87%. At this point, some may be saying that a white lie isn’t a lie. That’s because you are looking at lying from the adult perspective--that lies are acceptable, when told with the intent of helping someone, or protecting another’s feelings. But kids don’t think of lying in the same way. For them, the intent behind a lie--for good or for ill--is irrelevant. It is so irrelevant that, for very young kids, you can’t even lie by accident. Someone who gives out wrong information, but believed it to be true, is still a liar in these kids’ book. Kids just don’t believe that lying comes in shades of white or gray. Lying is much simpler than that: lying is telling somebody something that isn’t so; lying is really bad; and lying gets you punished. And if it gets you punished, you shouldn’t do it. In Talwar’s lab, parents have literally cheered to hear their kids lie about how great it is to have received the old soap. The parents have pride over their children’s knowing the socially appropriate response. Talwar’s regularly amazed by this. The parents never even seem to realize that the child told a lie. They never want to scold the child afterwards, or talk about the kid’s behavior. Regardless of the parents’ pride, the kids aren’t happy about their successfully lying. Instead, it can be torture for them. I was at Talwar’s lab when she was doing a version of the unwanted gift experiment with kids in the first and second grades. Watching kid after kid react to that gross bar of soap, I could really see how emotionally difficult it is for kids to tell a white lie. The kids were disappointed when they were handed the soap, but that was nothing compared to the discomfort they showed while having to lie about liking it. They are uneasy. Some looked like they were going to cry. It was simply painful to watch. Indeed, Talwar has found that some kids just can’t even bring themselves to say something nice about the present. About 20% of 11-year-olds just refuse to tell a white lie about that unwanted gift--even after their parents encouraged them to do so. And about 14% of kids still won’t tell a white lie, even after their parents specifically explained the prosocial (亲社会的) reasons to tell the lie. These kids just can’t reconcile the disconnect between knowing how bad lying is, and being told they should now lie. Talwar cautions that we need to recognize that, at least from the kids’ point of view, white lies really are still lies. We should take care to explain the motivation behind the untruth--that we want to protect the other person’s feelings. Kids may still fail to completely understand the distinction, but at least it will encourage them to think about others’ feelings when they act. And we need to reassure children that they won’t be punished for a specific white lie--because they did something nice for someone else. Talwar also warns that we adults should pay attention to our own use of white lies. Kids notice these untruths-and that we rarely get punished for them. If kids believe that we regularly lie to get out of uncomfortable social situations, they are more likely to adopt a similar strategy of lying. If we don’t watch it, we could unintentionally be giving kids yet another present: a license to lie. From the kids’ point of view, lying ______.

A. is acceptable as long as it can help others
B. is not bad if it doesn’t get them punished
C. is a good way to protect other’s feeling
D. is not acceptable even it is a white lie

近年来,中介业发展十分迅速,留学、务工、房屋买卖不一而足,而劳务输出和留学方面存在的问题最多。许多“黑中介”、非法中介更是无孔不入,让人防不胜防。 (1) 2002年10月初,百年煤都抚顺,一声尖锐的警笛冲淡了节日的喜庆。经过严密的调查取证,警方以涉嫌组织妇女跨国卖淫罪,将犯罪嫌疑人魏风娟、王中秋两人逮捕。不久,涉案的另一犯罪嫌疑人王敏也在新疆乌鲁木齐市被抓获。在警方的刑讯室里,魏凤娟等人交代了他们的犯罪事实。 早年从抚顺市丝绸厂下岗的魏凤娟,不脚踏实地地挣钱养家糊口,却整天幻想着捞取不义之财而一夜暴富。当听说近几年抚顺有不少下岗女工受出国淘金热的影响,为了摆脱贫困不惜变卖家产争相出国的情况后,苦苦寻觅发财良机的魏凤娟就像被注入了一剂强心针,一个组织妇女跨国卖淫的罪恶念头在她心里产生。于是她找到狐朋狗友王中秋、王敏,合伙干起了“黑中介”。 2002年初,魏凤娟以自己在阿联酋有生意为名,通过朋友和亲属等关系,多方联系在家赋闲的妇女。魏凤娟首先对她们编造自己在阿联酋开饭店挣大钱的“辉煌经历”,告诉这些妇女到那里打工每月最低可赚4000-6000元人民币,她吹嘘自己经常介绍别人去国外打工,能办理全套出国手续。为了骗取他人信任,魏风娟甚至说,她经常要从阿联酋回来为饭店招服务员,月薪最少5000元,包吃包住,许多人抢着去。在淘金梦的诱惑下,9名妇女听信了魏凤娟的花言巧语,开始按她的要求四处筹集出国费用。 魏凤娟一伙根据她们的年龄和长相,向这9名妇女分别收取了1.6万-2.3万元不等的出国手续费用,为她们办理了护照和出国旅游签证等相关手续,王中秋购买了飞机票。5月14日晚,魏凤娟和王中秋带着她们来到了北京首都机场。可是,在飞机将要起飞的前1个小时,魏凤娟突然拿出9份事先写好的协议书。协议书写有:在国外发生一切意外与魏没有关系,然后魏凤娟让每位妇女在各自协议书的背面写上“自愿当小姐”字样,不签字不准上飞机。这些妇女一下子呆住了,出国打工还要过此一关吗尽管心有不愿,但考虑到家庭的困境,而且交了中介费,无奈她们只得在协议书上签了字。怀着侥幸心理,这9人跟随魏凤娟飞到了阿联酋的沙加机场。王敏同另一男子开着面包车在机场等候,她们被带到了王敏在阿联酋的住处。王敏当即强迫她们每人又交了200美元食宿费。随后,魏凤娟对这9名妇女直言道:“现在暂时没有别的活,要挣钱就得当小姐。”魏凤娟和王敏当即四处打电话或直接外出拉皮条,当晚就令这些妇女们接客。 (2) 2002年4月,一直失业在家的徐某通过朋友认识了魏凤娟。魏凤娟干练利落,对人热情。听说徐某下岗在家,她便想方设法与徐某接近,介绍她在阿联酋的经历。听魏凤娟说在阿联酋挣钱容易,徐某心动了。 4月下旬,魏凤娟带着徐某等人一起办理了护照。魏让徐某先交人民币9000元,之后又让她交了 1万。生活本已十分拮据的徐某狠了狠心,把自己两室一厅的房子卖了,勉强凑够了钱。徐某曾警觉地问过魏凤娟:“不会让我们做‘小姐’吧那样挣钱再多,我也不干。”“绝对不会!你们可以在我的饭店打工,我也可以介绍你们当保姆。”魏凤娟拍着胸脯保证。 5月15日下午,徐某登上了赴阿联酋的飞机。想着到那个神秘的国度,想着只要省吃俭用、努力工作,不用几年就可以回国过舒心日子…… 16日晚11点,飞机终于到达了阿联酋。 当晚,魏凤娟安排徐某等9个人睡上下铺挤在一个不到14平方米的屋子里,每人每个月还要交给房主500迪拉姆(阿联酋货币,合人民币1200元左右)。魏凤娟说要带她们到街上走走“见见世面”,便领她们上了街。大街上人不多,有的人看着徐某一行,目光却很怪异。一会儿,一个40岁左右的男人走了过来,魏凤娟上前搭讪,两个人聊了几句。随后,魏又走了回来,让她们等一下。过了约半个小时,魏凤娟回来了,手里还拿着几张迪拉姆,对徐某她们说:“看到了吗在这里挣钱就这么简单。”徐某一下子明白了过来,这不是让她们卖淫吗美梦顷刻间破灭,她连想死的心都有。 因为一直没有人上钩,魏风娟无奈将徐等人带了回来。“你们把剩下的钱退我吧,我不可能同意卖淫的,我要回国。”回到住处,徐对魏凤娟说。魏凤娟反而笑了:“来到这儿,你就不能做主了。你干也得干,不干也得干。保持纯洁有什么用,不干你等着饿死吧。”此后,徐每天呆呆地坐着,以泪洗面。听说有两个同来的人逃回了国,可是即使回国,房子没了,存款没了,还欠了那么多债,怎么见人…… 有几个姐妹希望找到正当的工作,可是,她们最终绝望了……魏凤娟几次来找徐,都被徐某坚决拒绝。又过了几天,魏凤娟忽然让徐她们搬家,刚安顿下来,魏凤娟就警告她们:“你们出入小心点,别几个人一起出去。”后来徐才知道,原来这里是魏凤娟安排的进行卖淫交易的一个窝点,还曾被当地警方端过,直到现在还有人经常来检查。这下,徐更不敢出门了,就算非出门不可,也必须把自己捂得严严实实的。“我没做什么亏心事,可是在那里,我就像做了贼,总觉得别人拿异样的眼光看着我,沉重的心理负担让我随时会发疯。”徐哽咽着说,“后来,珍珍(化名)非回国不可。她和我很好,我也下定决心:先回国,然后再解决钱的问题。” (3) “提起那段不堪回首的往事,我现在还心酸和害怕。”2002年27岁的大连瓦房店农家女张某偶然认识了到瓦房店办事的魏凤娟。魏当时说:去阿联酋的中国商城卖服装挣钱,月工资最少为4000- 6000元人民币。如果不卖服装,也可以在那里做保姆,且还包吃包住。为了打消张某的迟疑和顾虑,魏凤娟多次给她打电话称“我也去,你不用害怕,你跟我还能吃着亏吗”张也曾想过,那里的钱难道就那么好挣吗但在魏凤娟的再三催促下,想为家里多挣点钱的念头占了上风,就借了1.7万元钱后,在丈夫的陪同下,张来到了抚顺。魏风娟与弟弟、弟妹和另外一个40多岁的女人“热情”地接待了张某夫妇,还主动请他们到一家小饭店吃饭。魏凤娟拉着张某的手说:“没事,用不着担心,大姐帮你。再说了,咱们这么多人都能照顾你。”魏凤娟的弟妹也插话说:“放心去吧,这次我也去。她(魏凤娟)骗谁也不会骗自己的弟妹吧。” 张某在酒桌上左右为难,她妈妈先后6次从瓦房店挂长途到她丈夫的手机,哭着对她说:“孩儿啊,这钱可不能交呀!这么远出去打工,一定要慎重啊。”见张犹犹豫豫不肯交钱,魏凤娟从口袋里掏出一打纸说:“我办了这么多签证,她们现在在那边干得都很好,你怕什么”吃罢饭后,魏凤娟当着张的面给一个负责办理签证的人打电话:“等一等,这里还有一个马上要交钱的。”随后,问张:“你到底去不去 你现在交钱我马上给你办签证,再过十天八天我们就走了。”见时间如此紧迫,张狠狠心终于把钱交了。 就在他们要离开抚顺奔赴阿联酋的前一天,张的母亲和姐姐专程从大连赶来为她送行。她的妈妈心存顾虑地问魏风娟有没有什么危险,魏凤娟答道:“有什么危险大姑娘出门干点啥不挣钱呢”闻听此言,张母越想越不对劲儿,悄悄提醒女儿可别是在外面做“小姐”。张听罢,大哭,对魏凤娟说:“大姐,我不想去了!”此时的魏凤娟已换了一副嘴脸。“我现在是一分钱也退不出来。这事也不是我一个人办的,你的钱也不在我手里,全都交上去了。”就这样,万般无奈的张踏上了飞机。 来到阿联酋,当她幻想着马上工作时,却被告知是做“小姐”,张痛不欲生,当街大哭起来。感到面子上过不去的魏凤娟大骂她是个丧门星,装纯洁。“你再哭!让警察看见,我们都要被抓起来。”她吓得再也不敢哭了。在街上又等了两个多小时,没有人理睬她们。回到住处,张把自己关在屋里大哭了两天。 第三天,魏凤娟再次叫她出去接客,张死活不去,又哭了一天……最后她与另外两名被骗妇女设法返回了祖国。 (4) 在抚顺市一座破旧的楼房里,47岁的受害人李某紧闭双眼,躺在床上,呻吟不止。一直守护在床边的丈夫说,最近一周来李某精神失常,经常胡言乱语。“她左胳膊和左腿全折了,左肋骨折了五六根,右胳膊手术后还留下了刀疤,里面现在还打着钢板,后脑还有一个红肿大包……”李某的丈夫满脸疲惫,向笔者介绍说,为了出国,李先后东挪西凑了1.8万元,全部交给了那个叫魏凤娟的女人,怀着美梦来到阿联酋。可是,她不但没有从梦中的国度淘到金子,自己还变成神志不清的残废。 跟着魏凤娟到了阿联酋后,发现自己上当受骗的李某,偏偏又在六神无主的悲愤之际遭遇车祸。幸好阿联酋当地医院给中国驻阿联酋大使馆发来传真,要求中方出面,李某这才在中国大使馆人员的护送下回国治疗。李某7月初回国后,在抚顺市中医院住了一个多月院,花了1.3万多元医药费仍未治愈。“我们借了那么多债,哪有钱再住院啊。”为了减轻李某的病痛,现在家人只能给她用“扑热息痛”和“盐酸曲马多片”止痛。 笔者和她丈夫说话时,李突然睁开双眼,神色紧张地四下张望着,小声说:“别说话,他们来了……”“我要走了,远远地走了……”“白求恩来了,他来救我来了……”。 李某的父亲今年76岁了,患有脑血栓和小脑萎缩。为了救助女儿,他把自己的棺材板钱都拿了出来,现在也全部花光了…… 在租住的10余平方米的房间里,李某夫妇的全部家当是一个皮箱、一个电饭锅、一个暖水瓶和一个坏了的单卡录音机。她丈夫告诉笔者,其余的东西都是房东的,连每月50元房钱他都交不起……他现在真的是倾家荡产了。 请用不超过150字的篇幅,概括出给定资料所反映的主要问题。

答案查题题库